November 17, 2003. ת Alfred Kinsey.

National Review recently had an article on Alfred Kinsey. Kinsey was a professor here at Indiana University, and we still have a sex research institute named after him, to our shame. For it turns out that despite the facade of scientific objectivity that Kinsey displayed, of a staid entomologist who had decided to apply boringly rigorous statistical techniques to an important but taboo subject, Kinsey's research was laughable in its absence of rigor and he himself was a secret adulterer, homosexual, and masochist. The lack of rigor was obvious, and commented on, " even at the time. The opposition to sexual morality has come out only recently. But see how similar, despite their different spins, are articles in the conservative National Review and the liberal Nation on Kinsey. First, the National Review.

In terms of subjects, Kinsey used volunteers -- a practice that scholars decry because of the selection bias it introduces. Many psychologists say that exhibitionists and unconventional sexual experimenters are the most likely respondents, thus distorting the results of the studies. A quarter to nearly half of Kinsey's subjects were prisoners, hardly reflective of the general population. Plus, over 1,400 of his subjects were sex offenders. Kinsey's samples were skewed in other ways as well: His subjects were overwhelmingly single when less than a third of the population was single during the 1950s, and they were also predominantly college educated.

Perhaps the most offensive aspect of Kinsey's supposedly "scientific" method was his definitions. He classified prostitutes and cohabiting females as "married" women, and then claimed that 26 percent of married women committed adultery. Of course, his deceptive definition of "married" was buried in the details of the lengthy book and didn't end up in popular versions of the research.

Critics of Kinsey's methodology and findings include fellow sex researchers who are aware that their work is suspect because of Kinsey's malpractice. One was noted psychologist Abraham Maslow, whom Kinsey asked to validate the research. After studying it carefully, Maslow exposed the problems. Kinsey severed all contact with him and ended both their professional and personal relationships, refusing to acknowledge the criticism.

SPECIAL AGENDAS: In his personal life, Albert Kinsey was promiscuously bisexual, sado- masochistic, and a decadent voyeur who enjoyed filming his wife having sex with his staff. He was hostile to religion and hated taboos of any kind. He often expressed the opinion that all sex is good. He advocated sexual activity as early, as varied, and as long as possible, claiming that a child's sexual activity would prepare him for better sexual adjustment later in life. He sought to validate numerous sexual practices (homosexuality, adultery, bisexuality, pedophilia, incest, and bestiality) that were not generally accepted at the time. Many of those sexual activities are more common today -- in part a legacy of Kinsey's influence over American culture.

Kinsey was not an objective scientist but a passionate ideologue particularly interested in using his research to change laws. In 1958, he won a court case allowing him to import pornography for his sex studies. This decision struck down postal laws, of course, and opened the floodgates for international pornography.

Compare this with the Martin Duberman review in the Nation (November 3, 1997, pp. 40-43) of the book, ALFRED C. KINSEY: A Public/Private Life by James H. Jones. Duberman was the Distinguished Professor of History at CUNY, and the Kinsey Institute has posted this review on the web as a defence of Kinsey.
Jones presents evidence -- full, incontrovertible and previously known only to a small circle of insiders -- that Kinsey often had sex and occasionally fell in love with people of his own gender. Yet Jones also tells us that Kinsey was lovingly married for thirty-five years to Clara McMillen, and that their relationship was in no sense perfunctory, certainly not sexually. A decade into their marriage, Alfred and Clara were "eagerly" exploring various coital positions newly recommended by a friend; and they maintained a sexual relationship until Kinsey became ill near the end of his life. With Clara's knowledge, Kinsey also had sex with other women during their marriage -- as did Clara with other men.

...

Where another biographer might have emphasized Kinsey's remarkable capacity for open- minded exploration, Jones persists in negatively labeling unconventional sexual behavior as "skating near the edge," as "compulsive" and "addictive" risk-taking.

...

Elsewhere, Jones refers to Kinsey's "fa�ade of objectivity" -- as if value-free social science has ever existed, or been more than approximated as an ideal. Of course Kinsey's personal needs and motivations influenced his findings; this is primer stuff in social science.

Thus, it seems that even Kinsey's defenders admit that he secretly indulged in perverted sexual practices (I have not quoted the bits about how he liked to tie ropes around his private parts) and lacked objectivity. Rather, the modern defence is "So what?" and "This is a complicated subject, and morality is naive and simplistic."

I should add that Professor Duberman denies that Kinsey's statistics are worthless, directly contradicting other reports I have read on Kinsey (including the one above from National Review) and modern sex surveys. Duberman says:

...Paul Gebhard (one of Kinsey's co-authors and his successor as director of the Kinsey Institute for Sex Research -- he retired in 1982), himself reacting to criticism leveled against the two volumes, spent years "cleaning" the Kinsey data of its purported contaminants -- removing, for example, all material derived from prison populations in the basic sample. In 1979, Gebhard, with Alan Johnson, published The Kinsey Data, and -- to his own surprise -- found that Kinsey's original estimates held: Instead of Kinsey's 37 percent, Gebhard and Johnson came up with 36.4 percent; the 10 percent figure (with prison inmates excluded) came to 9.9 percent for white, college-educated males and 12.7 percent for those with less education. And as for the call for a "random sample," a team of independent statisticians studying Kinsey's procedures had concluded as far back as 1953 that the unique problems inherent in sex research precluded the possibility of obtaining a true random sample, and that Kinsey's interviewing technique had been "extraordinarily skillful."
JSTOR has the Cochran-Mosteller-Tukey report, which must be Duberman's "independent statisticians". My reading of their report is that it says Kinsey's misuse of statistics is enormous-- that nobody on his team knew much about statistics or thought to consult with statisticians, and they made completely unreasonable inferences-- but that they wanted to be gentle with Kinsey because they liked the fact that someone was now trying to collect data and they didn't want to discourage novices from at least trying to do statistical work. I take the same attitude myself when I comment on papers by untrained law professors trying to use economics or statistics-- the expert should not flaunt his expertise and tell the novice to go back to doing journalism and not to even try to use rigorous methods; he should gently correct mistakes and point the novice to how to do things better. Law professors do the same with me when I write papers that rely on fine points of the law. But reading between the lines, the CMT report says that Kinsey's conclusions about the US population in general are entirely worthless. See

" Statistical Problems of the Kinsey Report," William G. Cochran; Frederick Mosteller; John W. Tukey Journal of the American Statistical Association, Vol. 48, No. 264. (Dec., 1953), pp. 673-716.

[ permalink, http://php.indiana.edu/~erasmuse/w/03.11.17d.htm ]

To return to Eric Rasmusen's weblog, click http://php.indiana.edu/~erasmuse/w/0.rasmusen.htm.